Memoria de trabajo y Consciencia:

tres perspectivas teóricas

Visualizaciones: 382

Autores/as

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.30854/anf.v29.n53.2022.800

Palabras clave:

consciencia, memoria de trabajo, teoría del espacio de trabajo global, modelo jerárquico, yo

Resumen

La perspectiva de la Memoria de Trabajo (MT) como proceso consciente ha permitido definir la consciencia como el contenido de la MT; sin embargo, han surgido inquietudes sobre las comparaciones que se han realizado entre ambas. Objetivo: aunque la relación entre estos dos campos de estudio ha sido planteada desde la psicología, la filosofía y la neurociencia, una revisión teórica que aborde los elementos centrales de las perspectivas más citadas enriquecería el debate en esta área de conocimiento. Metodología: esta revisión se centra en tres perspectivas teóricas: 1) el modelo multicomponente de memoria de trabajo; 2) la Teoría del Espacio de Trabajo Global (GWT); 3) el modelo jerárquico. Se analizaron 113 artículos en los que se abordaron las tres perspectivas anteriores. Resultados: el modelo multicomponente de memoria de trabajo aporta una descripción funcional básica sobre cómo las representaciones mentales permanecen en línea durante un procesamiento cognitivo complejo. De este modo, de un lado, el intercambio de información entre el ejecutivo central y el búfer episódico; y de otro lado, el bucle fonológico y la agenda visuoespacial se presenta a través del procesamiento consciente. Conclusiones: asimismo, el ejecutivo central controla y modifica la atención, pero el búfer episódico permite la disponibilidad de información multimodal.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Métricas

Cargando métricas ...

Citas

Aleksander, I. y Gamez, D. (2011). Informational Theories of Consciousness: A Review and Extension. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 718(1), 139-147. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-0164-3_12

Alloway., T., Gathercole, S., Adams, A., Willis, C., Eaglen, R. y Lamont, E. (2005). Working Memory and Phonological Awareness as Predictors of Progress Towards Early Learning Goals at School Entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 417–426. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26804

Andrade, J. (Ed). (2002). Working Memory in Perspective. Taylor Fracis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203194157

Andrs, P. y Van der Linden, M. (2001). Supervisory Attentional System in Patients with Focal Frontal Lesions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(2), 225-239. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.2.225.1212

Angelopoulou, E. y Drigas, A. (2021). Working Memory, Attention and their Relationship: A theoretical Overview. Research. Society and Development,10(5), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.33448/rsd-v10i5.15288

Atkinson, R. y Shiffrin, R. (2016). Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes. En R.J. Sternberg, S.T Fiske y D.J., Foss (Eds.), Scientists Making a Difference: One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about their Most Important Contributions. (pp. 115-118). Cambridge University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316422250.025

Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. y Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-Down versus Bottom-up Attentional Control: A Failed Theoretical Dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437-443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010

Baars, B. (1988). A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. In A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge University Press.

Baars, B. (2005). Global Workspace Theory of Consciousness: Toward a Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Experience. Progress in Brain Research, 150(1), 45-53. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9

Baars, B. y Franklin, S. (2003). How Conscious Experience and Working Memory Interact. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 166-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00056-1

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working Memory. Science, 255(5044), 556–559. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1736359

Baddeley, A. (2000). The Episodic Buffer: A New Component of Working Memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. https://www.cell.com/trends/cognitivesciences/fulltext/S1364-6613(00)01538-2_returnURL=https%3A%2F%2Flinkinghub.elsevier.com%2Fretrieve%2Fpii%2FS1364661300015382%3Fshowall%3Dtrue

Baddeley, A. (2003). Working Memory: Looking Back and Looking Forward. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201

Baddeley, A. (2007). Working Memory, Thought, and Action. Oxford Psychology Series. https://books.google.com.co/bookshl=es&lr=&id=DRIeAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PT14&dq=).+Working+Memory,+Thought,+and+Action.+In+Working+memory,+thought,+and+action&ots=XsB0L2tJc7&sig=vZTNVXC48z8HzkmZSOKnm88xM#v=onepage&q=).%20Working%20Memory%2C%20Thought%2C%20and%20Action.%20In%20Working%20memory%2C%20thought%2C%20and%20action&f=false

Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014

Baddeley, A. (2017). Exploring Working Memory. Taylor and Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315111261

Baddeley, A., Allen, R. y Hitch, G. (2010). Investigating the Episodic Buffer. Psychologica Belgica, 50(3-4), 223-243. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb-50-3-4-223

Baumeister, R. y Masicampo, E. (2010). Conscious Thought is for Facilitating Social and Cultural Interactions: How Mental Simulations Serve the Animal-culture Interface. Psychological Review, 117(3), 945-971. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019393

Bergström, F. y Eriksson, J. (2014). Maintenance of Non-consciously Presented Information Engages the Prefrontal Cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 938. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00938

Broadway, J. y Engle, R. (2011). Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Temporal Discrimination. PLoS ONE, 6(10), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025422

Brown, E., Lydic, R. y Schiff, N. (2010). General Anesthesia, Sleep, and Coma. New England Journal of Medicine, 363,2638-2650. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra0808281

Buchsbaum, B. (2013). The Role of Consciousness in the Phonological Loop: Hidden in Plain Sight. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(1), 1-5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00496

Burgess, P. y Stuss, D. (2017). Fifty Years of Prefrontal Cortex Research: Impact on Assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(9-10), 755-767. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000704

Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual Attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13),1484-1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012

Chica, A. , Bartolomeo, P. y Lupiáñez, J. (2013). Two Cognitive and Neural Systems for Endogenous and Exogenous Spatial Attention. Behavioural Brain Research, 237(1),107-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.027

Corbetta, M. y Shulman, G. (2002). Control of Goal-directed and Stimulusdriven Attention in the Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755

Corbetta, M., Patel, G. y Shulman, G. (2008). The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

Cowan, N. (2012). An Embedded-Processes Model of Working Memory. En A.,Miyake y P. Shah (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (pp. 62-101). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139174909.006

Crick, F. y Koch, C. (1990). Towards a Neurobiological Theory of Consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2(1), 263-265. https://authors.library.caltech.edu/40352/1/148.pdf

Crick, F. y Koch, C. (2003). A Framework for Consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119-126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn0203-119

De Loof, E., Verguts, T., Fias, W. y Van Opstal, F. (2013). Opposite Effects of Working Memory on Subjective Visibility and Priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(6), 1959-1965. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033093

Dehaene, S. y Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200-227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018

Dehaene, S. y Naccache, L. (2001).Towards a Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness: Basic Evidence and a Workspace Framework. Cognition, 79(1-2), 1-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2

Del Cul, A., Baillet, S. y Dehaene, S. (2007). Brain Dynamics Underlying the Nonlinear Threshold for Access to Consciousness. PLoS Biology, 5(10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050260

Duncan, J. (2013). The Structure of Cognition: Attentional Episodes in Mind and Brain. Neuron, 80(1), 35-50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.015

Earl, B. (2014). The Biological Function of Consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00697

Eimer, M. y Grubert, A. (2014). Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Current Biology, 24(2), 193-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.001

Engle, R. (2002). Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19-23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160

Fingelkurts, A. y Fingelkurts, A. (2017). Information Flow in the Brain: Ordered Sequences of Metastable States. Information, 8(22), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3390/info8010022

Fingelkurts, A., Fingelkurts, A. y Neves, C. (2010). Natural World Physical, Brain Operational, and Mind Phenomenal Space-time. Physics of Life Reviews, 7(2), 195-249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2010.04.001

Fingelkurts, A., Fingelkurts, A., Krause, C., Kaplan, A., Borisov, S. y Sams, M. (2003). Structural (Operational) Synchrony of EEG Alpha Activity During an Auditory Memory Task. NeuroImage, 20(1), 529-542. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00305-7

Fries, P., Roelfsema, P., Engel, A., König, P. y Singer, W. (1997). Synchronization of Oscillatory Responses in Visual Cortex Correlates with Perception in Interocular Rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(23), 12699-12704.

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699

Frith, C. (2005). The Quest for Consciousness: A Neurobiological Approach. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 407. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.407

Gayet, S., Paffen, C. y Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). Information Matching the Content of Visual Working Memory Is Prioritized forConscious Access. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2472-2480.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613495882

Gray, C., König, P., Engel, A. y Singer, W. (1989). Oscillatory Responses in Cat Visual Cortex Exhibit Inter-columnar Synchronization which Reflects Global Stimulus Properties. Nature, 338(6213), 334-337. https://doi.org/10.1038/338334a0

Hassin, R., Bargh, J., Engell, A. y McCulloch, K. (2009). Implicit Working Memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 665-678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.003

Herculano-Houzel, S., Munk, M., Neuenschwander, S. y Singer, W. (1999). Precisely Synchronized Oscillatory Firing Patterns Require Electroencephalographic Activation. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 3992-4010. https://www.jneurosci.org/content/19/10/3992

Hermes, D., Miller, K., Wandell, B. y Winawer, J. (2015). Stimulus Dependence of Gamma Oscillations in Human Visual Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 2951-2959. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu091

Imas, O., Ropella, K., Ward, B., Wood, J. y Hudetz, A. (2005). Volatile Anesthetics Disrupt Frontal-posterior Recurrent Information Transfer at Gamma Frequencies in Rats. Neuroscience Letters, 387(3), 145-150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.018

Jiang, Y., Costello, P. y He, S. (2007). Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression. Psychological Science, 18(4), 349-355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x

Jonkisz, J. (2016). Subjectivity: A Case of Biological Individuation and an Adaptive Response to Informational Overflow. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(1), 1-6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01206

Kertai, M., Whitlock, E. y Avidan, M. (2012). Brain Monitoring with Electroencephalography and the Electroencephalogram–Derived Bispectral Index During Cardiac Surgery. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 114(3),533-546. https://doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0b013e31823ee030

Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M. y Tononi, G. (2016). Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Progress and Problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307-321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22

Kolb, B. y Whishaw, I. (2006). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Seminars in Hearing, 27(3), 127,135. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2006-947280

Laureys, S., Owen, A. y Schiff, N. (2004). Brain Function in Coma, Vegetative State, and Related Disorders. Lancet Neurology, 3(9), 537,546. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X

Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and Confused?: Selective Attention Under Load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75-82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004

Libet, B., Wright, E. y Gleason, C. (1982). Readiness-potentials Preceding Unrestricted “Spontaneous” vs. Pre-planned Voluntary Acts. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 54(3), 322-335. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(82)90181-X

Lisman, J. y Jensen, O. (2013). The Theta-Gamma Neural Code. Neuron, 77(6), 1002-1016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007

Logie, R. y Pearson, D. (1997). The Inner Eye and the Inner Scribe of Visuo-spatial Working Memory: Evidence from Developmental Fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 241-257. https://doi.org/10.1080/713752559

Melloni, L., Molina, C., Pena, M., Torres, D., Singer, W. y Rodriguez, E. (2007). Synchronization of Neural Activity across Cortical Areas Correlates with Conscious Perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(11), 2858-2865. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4623-06.2007

Melloni, L., Schwiedrzik, C., Müller, N., Rodriguez, E. y Singer, W. (2011). Expectations Change the Signatures and Timing of Electrophysiological Correlates of Perceptual Awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 1386- 1396. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4570-10.2011

Monto, S. (2012). Nested Synchrony–A Novel Cross-scale Interaction Among Neuronal Oscillations. Frontiers in Physiology, 3(1), 1-7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2012.00384

Morsella, E., Berger, C. y Krieger, S. (2011). Cognitive and Neural Components of the Phenomenology of Agency. Neurocase, 17(3), 209-230. https://doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2010.504727

Moruzzi, G. y Magoun, H. (1949). Brain Stem Reticular Formation and Activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1(1-4), 455-473. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(49)90219-9

Murphy, M., Bruno, M., Riedner, B., Boveroux, P., Noirhomme, Q., Landsness, E., Brichant, J., Phillips, C., Massimini, M. Laureys, S., Tononni, G. y Boly, M. (2011). Propofol Anesthesia and

Sleep: A High-density EEG Study. Sleep, 34(3), 283-291.https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/34.3.283

Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a Working Memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51(1), 45–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51002-X

Pan, Y., Lin, B., Zhao, Y. y Soto, D. (2014). Working Memory Biasing of Visual Perception without Awareness. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(7), 2051-2062. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0566-2

Picton, T., Stuss, D., Alexander, M., Shallice, T., Binns, M. y Gillingham, S. (2007). Effects of focal frontal lesions on response inhibition. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 826-838. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhk031

Pitts, M., Metzler, S. y Hillyard, S. (2014). Isolating Neural Correlates of Conscious Perception from Neural Correlates of Reporting one’s Perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1), 1-16.

https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01078

Pockett, S. y Holmes, M. (2009). Intracranial EEG Power Spectra and Phase Synchrony during Consciousness and Unconsciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 1049-1055.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.010

Purdon, P., Pierce, E., Mukamel, E., Prerau, M., Walsh, J., Wong, K., Salazar-Gómez, A., Harrell, P., Sampson, A., Cimenser, A., Ching, S., Kopell, N., Tavares-Stoeckel, C., Habeeb, K., Merhar, R. y Brown, E. N. (2013). Electroencephalogram Signatures of Loss and Recovery of consciousness from Propofol. Proceedings of the National Academy

of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(12), 1142-1151.

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221180110

Railo, H., Koivisto, M. y Revonsuo, A. (2011). Tracking the Processes behind Conscious Perception: A Review of Event-related Potential Correlates of Visual Consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 972-983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.019

Ray, S. y Maunsell, J. (2011). Network Rhythms Influence the Relationship between Spike- triggered Local Field Potential and Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(35), 12674-12682. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1856-11.2011

Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J., Martinerie, J., Renault, B. y Varela, F. (1999). Perception’s shadow: Long-distance Synchronization of Human Brain Activity. Nature, 397(6718), 430-433. https://doi.org/10.1038/17120

Roelfsema, P., Engel, A., König, P. y Singer, W. (1997). Visuomotor Integration is Associated with Zero Time-lag Synchronization Among Cortical Areas. Nature, 385(6612), 157-161. https://doi.org/10.1038/385157a0

Schiff, N., Nauvel, T. y Victor, J. (2014). Large-scale Brain Dynamics in Disorders of Consciousness. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25(1), 7-14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.10.007

Schraw, G. y Dennison, R. (1994). Assessing Metacognitive Awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19(4), 460-475. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1994.1033

Schraw, G. y Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive Theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7(4), 351-371. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02212307

Sergent, C., Baillet, S. y Dehaene, S. (2005). Timing of the Brain Events Underlying Access to Consciousness during the Attentional Blink. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1391-1400. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1549

Shepard, R. y Chipman, S. (1970). Second-order Isomorphism of Internal Representations: Shapes of states. Cognitive Psychology, 1(1), 1-17.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(70)90002-2

Siclari, F., LaRocque, J., Postle, B. y Tononi, G. (2013). Assessing SleepConsciousness within Subjects Using a Serial Awakening Paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542

Silverstein, B., Snodgrass, M., Shevrin, H. y Kushwaha, R. (2015). P3b, Consciousness, and

Complex Unconscious Processing. Cortex, 73(1), 216-227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.004

Singer, W. (1999). Time as Coding Space? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(2), 189-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80026-9

Smart, J. (1995). Sensations and Brain Processes. Behavioural Brain Research, 71(1-2), 157-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(95)00033-X

Soto, D., Mäntylä, T. y Silvanto, J. (2011). Working Memory without Consciousness. Current Biology, 21(22). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.049

Steriade, M. (2000). Corticothalamic Resonance, States of Vigilance and Mentation. Neuroscience, 101(2), 243-276. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00353-5

Steriade, M., Timofeev, I. y Grenier, F. (2001). Natural Waking and Sleep States: A View from Inside Neocortical Neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85(5),1969,1985. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969

Stuss, D. (2011). Functions of the Frontal Lobes: Relation to Executive Functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,17(5), 759-765. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000695

Stuss, D. (1991). Self, Awareness, and the Frontal Lobes: A Neuropsychological Perspective. En J. Strauss y G.R., Goethals (Eds.), The Self: Interdisciplinary Approaches (pp. 255-278). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-8264-5_13

Stuss, D. y Alexander, M. (2005). Does Damage to the Frontal Lobes Produce Impairment in Memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 84-88. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00340.x

Stuss, D. y Knight, R. (Eds). (2009). Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford Scholarship Online. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.001.0001

Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J. y John, E. (1965). Evoked-potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty. Science, 150(3700), 1187-1188. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.150.3700.1187

Tononi, G. (2012). Integrated Information Theory of Consciousness: An Updated Account. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 150(2-3). http://architalbiol.org/index.php/aib/article/view/15056

Tononi, G. y Koch, C. (2015). Consciousness: Here, There and Everywhere? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 370(1668), 2-18. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0167

Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M. y Koch, C. (2016). Integrated Information Theory: From Consciousness to its Physical Substrate. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(7), 450-461. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.44

Tsuchiya, N. y Koch, C. (2005). Continuous Flash Suppression Reduces Negative Afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096-1101. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1500

Unsworth, N. y Engle, R. (2007). The Nature of Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity: Active Maintenance in Primary Memory and Controlled Search from Secondary Memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104

Velichkovsky, B. (2017). Consciousness and Working Memory: Current Trends and Research Perspectives. Consciousness and Cognition, 55(1), 35-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.07.005

Wyart, V. y Tallon-Baudry, C. (2008). Neural Dissociation Between Visual Awareness and Spatial Attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(10), 2667- 2679. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4748-07.2008

Zeman, A. (1996). Attentional Processing. The Brain’s Art of Mindfulness. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 60(5). 596. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.60.5.596

Publicado

2022-07-05

Cómo citar

Landinez-Martínez, D.-A., Montoya-Arenas, D.-A., & Pineda-Salazar, D.-A. (2022). Memoria de trabajo y Consciencia:: tres perspectivas teóricas. ÁNFORA, 29(53), 244–272. https://doi.org/10.30854/anf.v29.n53.2022.800